Psy 342 Learning & Memory
Reconstructive Memory—Chapter 10 

I. Flashbulb Memory 

       A. Definition  

       Brown and Kulik (1977)—asked questions about 10 dramatic events, and observed that many of them were accompanied by a very vivid recollection of the receipt of the news. 

These memories form for very surprising and consequential events and are permanent, vivid representations of the event.  

Common details preserved--where they were when the event happened, who told them, who was with them, and what their emotional reaction was.  

 

B. Conway (1995) 

Event—Margaret Thatcher resignation 
Tested after 2 weeks and 1 year
Respondents were British, American, or Danish    

C. Neisser and Harsch (1992)  

Event—space shuttle explosion 

N = 106  

2 Tests—within 24 hours after event and almost 3 years later 

Poor accuracy--only 2.95 correct attributes out of 7

 High confidence ratings (4.17 out of 5), vivid recollections, but very inaccurate.    

D. Methodological Issues

 Long lag between event and initial test could obscure memory distortion

 Ss construct details to make a good story

 Lack of control for the emotion accompanying flashbulb events

 Weaver (1993) used a control memory

and found that accuracy decreased according to the forgetting function for both the flashbulb and control event.   

E. Are flashbulb memories special? 

Superior accuracy? 

Special processes? 

Finer detail?

 

II. Schemas  

A. Definition and background 

A configuration of general knowledge about objects and events. 

A schema usually includes subschemas. 

People’s expectations are based on experience--personal or vicarious    

B. Script--A schema describing a typical sequence of events, such as attending a lecture or going to a restaurant.  

 Scripts and schemas are generic; they describe typical events and allow for some variability (e.g., restaurants differ, from coffee shops to five-star establishments).  

 C. Inferences

 Based on script, listeners/readers infer actions not explicitly stated in a text or conversation.

 For instance, we have no difficulty understanding:

John went to a restaurant.

He ordered chicken.

He left a large tip.    

D. Bartlett (1932) 

lRejected popular view that memory and forgetting can be studied by means of nonsense syllables.

 

Argued for using rich and meaningful material under naturalistic conditions
Methods— Subjects were given a short story, or other material.  They read it and then free-recalled what they could remember after a certain period of time (retention interval). 

War of the Ghosts--one of the stories Bartlett used in his research

Examined the contents of the reproduced version of the stories to answer the following questions:
 
1.  What was recalled accurately?

 

2.  What was not recalled accurately?

 

3.  What do these results imply about the reproductive nature of memory?

4.  What do these results imply about the reconstructive nature of memory?

 

E.  Problems for schema theory

Both memory distortions and correct recall have been attributed to the schema.  

Schemas are so flexible that they can account for almost any data.

 

III. Eyewitness Memory    
Penny memory

A. Misinformation effect
In general--
People first view an event and afterward are given misleading information about the event. Later, they incorrectly recall the misleading information, rather than the event they actually saw (Zaragoza et al, 1997). 

Resembles retroactive interference.

 

But first Loftus showed that participants' memories for the original incident can even be influenced by subtle cues in the way they are questioned

 Loftus & Palmer (1974)—Preliminary study showed that the way a question is asked influences subsequent recall. 

“How fast were the cars going when they _____ each other?”

 Different verb intensity—smashed, collided, bumped, hit, contacted 

 

Intensity of verb influenced Ss estimate of speed

 Loftus, Miller, and Burns (1978)-- the classic experiment on the misinformation effect.

 Ss saw a series of slides. A sports car stopped at an intersection, and then it turned and hit a pedestrian.  Half saw yield sign at the intersection; the other half saw stop sign. 

Critical question contained information that was either consistent with a detail in the original slide series, inconsistent with that detail, or neutral (i.e., did not mention the detail).  

Ss were then shown two slides (stop sign or yield sign) and asked to select which they had previously seen. 

People who saw the inconsistent information were much less accurate than people in the other two conditions. 

Explanations:  

(1) Source Monitoring 

(2) Memory is active and constructive-- we construct a memory by combining/blending information from a variety of sources. 

(3) Consistency bias

Application: A confident eyewitness may not necessarily be an accurate eyewitness.

  video

B. Factors affecting accuracy of eyewitness testimony     take an eyewitness test 

Errors are More Likely If: 

(1) The witness’s attention has been distracted at the time of the event. 

(2) The misinformation is plausible. 

(3) There is social pressure to provide a specific answer.

(4) Eyewitness has been given positive feedback.

 (5) The response is based on process of comparison or elimination, rather than just "knowing"

 (6) Simultaneous line-up procedure (encourages relative judgments)

    What's the alternative?

 

IV.  Recovered Memory vs. False Memory Controversy  

A. Recovered memory perspective 

Memories can be forgotten and then recovered, e.g., sexual abuse during childhood    

B. False memory perspective 

 These researchers deny the accuracy of many reports about sudden recovery of early memories.

 Propose that many of these recovered memories are actually incorrect memories--constructed stories about events that never occurred.    

C. Laboratory research
Try this activity

 Researchers have demonstrated that people can create a false memory for a word they have not actually seen.   

Ss are asked to remember a list of words they had seen earlier. Accuracy is then measured.

 Roediger and McDermott (1995) found a false recall rate of 55%.

 People may “recall” events that are related to their actual experiences, but these events never really occurred.

 Ss can construct false memories for events in their own lives that never actually happened.

  Hyman and colleagues (1995) asked parents of Ss to supply information about several events.

 Interviewed each student individually about several events that really did occur.  Also, during the interview, researchers also planted several false memories.

 Some students gradually began to create a false memory (25% of Ss overall).    

D. Low ecological validity with respect to memory for childhood sexual abuse.

 

Story